Context dependency as a Function of Prediction Error-Based Attention

  1. Rosas, Juan M.
  2. Nelson, James Byron
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2019

Volumen: 40

Número: 2

Páginas: 34-45

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.2478/PSICOLJ-2019-0003 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

Abstract Context dependence of information has been shown to be based, at least in part, on the attention contexts received at the time of training. Recent research suggests that attention to irrelevant contexts may be a byproduct of the activation of a general exploratory attentional mechanism prompted by high prediction errors associated with situations of uncertainty. Alternatively, low prediction errors may engage an attentional mechanism of exploitation in situations in which contexts play a relevant role. A selective review discusses the potential of this approach to explain context switch effects from an attentional perspective.

Información de financiación

* Corresponding author: Juan M. Rosas. Department of Psychology. University of Jaen, SPAIN. E-mail: jmrosas@ujaen.es. Acknowledgements: The work and its presentation here were supported by Grants PSI2014-52263-C2-1-P and PSI2014-52263-C2-2-P from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Competitiveness. Correspondence concerning to this article may be addressed to Juan M. Rosas, jmrosas@ujaen.es or James Byron Nelson, Jamesbyron.nelson@ehu.es

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Abad M. J. Ramos-Álvarez M. M. & Rosas J. M. (2009). Partial reinforcement and context switch effects in human predictive learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology62 174-188. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701855561
  • Alcalá J. A. Callejas-Aguilera J. E. Lamoureux J. A. & Rosas J. M. (2019). Discrimination reversal facilitates subsequent acquisition of temporal discriminations in rats’ appetitive conditioning. Manuscript under review.
  • Alcalá J. A. Callejas-Aguilera J. E. Nelson B. & Rosas J. M. (2019). Reversal training facilitates acquisition of new learning in a Morris water maze. Manuscript under review.
  • Aristizabal J. A. Ramos-Álvarez M. M. Callejas-Aguilera J. E. & Rosas J. M. (2016). Attention to irrelevant contexts decreases as training increases: Evidence from eye-fixations in a human predictive learning task. Behavioural Processes124 66-73. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.12.008
  • Beesley T. Hanafi G. Vadillo M. A. Shanks D. R. & Livesey E. J. (2018). Overt attention in contextual cuing of visual search is driven by the attentional set but not by the predictiveness of distractors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 44 707-721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000467
  • Beesley T. Nguyen K. P. Pearson D. & Le Pelley M. E. (2015). Uncertainty and predictiveness determine attention to cues during human associative learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology68 2175-2199. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1009919
  • Bernal-Gamboa R. Callejas-Aguilera J. E. Nieto J. & Rosas J. M. (2013). Extinction Makes Conditioning Time-Dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes39 221-232. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0558-1
  • Bernal-Gamboa R. Nieto J. & Rosas J. M. (2015). Context specificity of taste aversion is boosted by pre-exposure and conditioning with a different taste. Behavioural Processes120 111-115. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.09.008
  • Bernal-Gamboa R. Rosas J. M. & Callejas-Aguilera J. E. (2014). Experiencing extinction within a task makes non extinguished information learned within a different task context-dependent. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21 803-808. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0558-1
  • Bernal-Gamboa R. Rosas J. M. & Nieto J. (2018). Extinction makes acquisition context-specific in conditioned taste aversion regardless of the context where acquisition and testing take place. Journal of Experimental Psychology.Animal Learning and Cognition 44(4) 385-395. https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000183
  • Bouton M. E. (1993). Context time and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin114 80-99. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80
  • Bouton M. E. (1994). Conditioning remembering and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes20 219-231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.20.3.219
  • Bouton M. E. (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. In M. S. Fanselow & M. E. Bouton (Eds.) Learning motivation and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385-409). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10223-019
  • Bouton M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory 11 485-494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.78804
  • Bouton M. E. & Bolles R. C. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation 10 445-466. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(79)90057-2
  • Bouton M. E. & Brooks D. C. (1993). Time and context effects on performance in a pavlovian discrimination reversal. Journal of Experimental Psychology 19 165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.19.2.165
  • Bouton M. E. Kenney F. A. & Rosengard C. (1990). State-dependent fear extinction with two benzodiazepine tranquilizers. Behavioral Neuroscience 104 44-55. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.104.1.44
  • Bouton M. E. & Peck C. A. (1989). Context effects on conditioning extinction and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation. Animal Learning and Behavior 17 188-198. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03207634
  • Bouton M. E. & Ricker S. T. (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior 22 317-324. http://doi:10.3758/BF03209840
  • Bouton M. E. & Sunsay C. (2001). Contextual control of appetitive conditioning: Influence of a contextual stimulus generated by a partial reinforcement procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology54B 109-125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000190
  • Bouton M. E. Todd T. P. Vurbic D. & Winterbauer N. (2011). Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior. Learning & Behavior 39 57-67. http://dx.doi.org//10.3758/s13420-011-0018-6
  • Bower G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American psychologist36 129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.36.2.129
  • Callejas-Aguilera J. E. Cubillas C. P. & Rosas J. M. (2019). Attentional Instructions Modulate Differential Context-Switch Effects after Short and Long Training in Human Predictive Learning. Manuscript under review.
  • Callejas-Aguilera J. E. & Rosas J. M. (2010). Ambiguity and context processing in human predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes36 482-494. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0018527
  • Cañadas E. Rodríguez-Bailón R. Milliken B. & Lupiáñez J. (2013). Social Categories as a Context for the Allocation of Attentional Control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General142 934-943. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0029794
  • Cobos P. L. González-Martín E. Varona-Moya S. & López F. J. (2013). Renewal effects in interference between outcomes as measured by a cued response reaction time task: Further evidence for associative retrieval models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 39 299-310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033528
  • Effting M. & Kindt M. (2007). Contextual control of human fear associations in a renewal paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45 2002.
  • Darby R. J. & Pearce J. M. (1995). Effects of context on responding during a compound stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 21 143-154. http://doi:10.1037/0097-7403.21.2.143
  • García-Gutiérrez A. & Rosas J. M. (2003). Context change as the mechanism of reinstatement in causal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes29 292-310. http://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.29.4.292
  • George D. N. & Kruschke J. K. (2012). Contextual modulation of attention in human category learning. Learning & Behaviour 40 530-541. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0072-8
  • Godden D. R. & Baddeley A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of psychology66 325-331. http://doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
  • Grillon C. Alvarez R. P. Johnson L. & Chavis C. (2008). Contextual specificity of extinction of delay but not trace eyeblink conditioning in humans. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor N.Y.) 15 387-389. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.855708
  • Hall G. & Channel S. (1985). Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes11470- 481. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.11.3.470
  • Hall G. & Rodriguez G. (2010). Associative and nonassociative processes in latent inhibition: An elaboration of the Pearce-Hall model. Latent inhibition: Cognition neuroscience and applications to schizophrenia 114-136. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511730184.007
  • Kraemer P. J. & Roberts. W. A (1984). The influence of flavor preexposure and test interval on conditioned taste aversions in the rat. Learning and Motivation15 259- 278. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(84)90022-5
  • Lattal K. M. Mullen M. T. & Abel T. (2003). Extinction renewal and spontaneous recovery of a spatial preference in the water maze. Behavioral Neuroscience117 1017–1028. http://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.1017
  • Le Pelley M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 57B 193–243. http://doi.org/10.1080/02724990344000141
  • Le Pelley M.E. Haselgrove M. & Esber G.R. (2012). Modeling attention in associative learning: Two processes or one? Learning & Behavior 40 292-304. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0084-4
  • Le Pelley M.E. Mitchell C.J. Beesley T. George D.N. & Wills A. J. (2016). Attention and associative learning in humans: An integrative Review. Psychological Bulletin 142 1111-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000064
  • León S. P. Abad M. J. F. & Rosas J. M. (2010). Giving contexts informative value makes information context-specific. Experimental Psychology57 46-53. http://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000006
  • León S. P. Abad M. J. F. & Rosas J. M. (2011). Context-outcome associations mediate context-switch effects in a human predictive learning task. Learning and Motivation42 84-98. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.10.001
  • Liberman A. M. (1951). A comparison of transfer effects during acquisition and extinction of two instrumental responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology 41 192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0062259Google Scholar
  • Lucke S. Lachnit H. Koenig S. & Uengoer M. (2013). The informational value of contexts affects context-dependent learning. Learning & Behavior41 285–297. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-013-0104-z
  • Lucke S. Lachnit H. Stüttgen M. C. & Uengoer M. (2014). The impact of context relevance during extinction learning. Learning & Behavior42 256-269. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-014-0143-0
  • Mackintosh N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review 82 276-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076778
  • Nelson J. B. (2002). Context specificity of excitation and inhibition in ambiguous stimuli. Learning and Motivation 33 284-310. http://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.2001.1112
  • Nelson J. B. (2009). Contextual control of first- and second-learned excitation and inhibition in equally ambiguous stimuli. Learning & Behavior 37 95-106. http://doi.org/10.3758/LB.37.1.95
  • Nelson J. B. & Lamoureux J. A. (2015). Contextual control of conditioning is not affected by extinction in a behavioral task with humans. Learning & Behavior 43 163-178. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0170-5.
  • Nelson J. B. Lombas S. & Léon S. P. (2011). Concurrent extinction does not render appetitive conditioning context specific. Learning & Behavior39 87-94. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-011-0023-9
  • Nelson J.B. & Sanjuan M.C. (2006). A context-specific latent inhibition effect in a human conditioned suppression task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology59 1003-1020. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210500417738
  • Nelson J. B. Sanjuan M. D. C. Vadillo-Ruiz S. Pérez J. & León S. P. (2011). Experimental renewal in human participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes37 58–70. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0020519
  • Neumann D. L. (2007). The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans. Learning and Motivation38 105-127. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2006.11.002
  • Nowak A. Werka T. & Knapska E. (2013). Social modulation in extinction of aversive memories. Behavioural Brain Research238 200–205. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.031
  • Ogallar P. M. Ramos-Álvarez M. M. Alcalá J. A. Moreno-Fernández M. M. & Rosas J. M. (2017). Attentional perspectives on context-dependence of information retrieval. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy17 121-136.
  • Pearce J.M. & Hall G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review87 532-552. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.532
  • Pearce J. M. & Mackintosh N. J. (2010). Two theories of attention: A review and a possible integration. In C. J. Mitchell & M. E. Le Pelley (Eds.) Attention and Associative Learning: From Brain to Behaviour (pp. 11-40) New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Peck C. A. & Bouton M. E. (1990). Context and performance in aversive-to-appetitive and appetitive-to-aversive transfer. Learning and Motivation21 1-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(90)90002-6
  • Preston G. C. Dickinson A. & Mackintosh N. J. (1986). Contextual conditional discriminations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology38 217-237. http://doi.10.1080/14640748608402230
  • Rescorla R. A. (2008). Within-subject renewal in sign tracking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology61 1793-1802. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210701790099.
  • Rosas J. M. & Bouton M. E. (1998). Context change and retention interval can have additive rather than interactive effects after taste aversion extinction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review5 79–83. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209459
  • Rosas J. M. & Callejas-Aguilera J. E. (2006). Context switch effects on acquisition and extinction in human predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition32 461-74. http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.461.
  • Rosas J. M. Callejas-Aguilera J. E Ramos-Álvarez M. M. & Abad M. J. (2006). Revision of Retrieval Theory of Forgetting: What does Make Information Context-Specific? International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy6 147-166.
  • Rosas J. M. García-Gutiérrez A. & Callejas-Aguilera J. E. (2006). Effects of context change upon retrieval of first and second-learned information in human predictive learning. Psicológica 27 35-56.
  • Rosas J. M. Vila N. J. Lugo M. & López L. (2001). Combined effect of context change and retention interval upon interference in causality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 27 153–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.27.2.153
  • Schmajuk N. A. Lam Y. W. & Gray J. A. (1996). Latent inhibition: A neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes22(3) 321- 349 https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.22.3.321
  • Shanab M. E. & Cotton J. W. (1970). Effects of runway training on behavior in the T-maze. Psychonomic Science 19(3) 129-130. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03335510
  • Smith S. M. (1979). Remembering in and out of context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory 5 460-471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.5.460
  • Thomas B. L. Larsen N. & Ayres J. J. B. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA ABC and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation 34 410-436. http://doi:10.1016/S0023-9690(03)00037-7
  • Thrailkill E. A. & Bouton M. E. (2017). Effects of outcome devaluation on instrumental behaviors in a discriminated heterogeneous chain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 43 88-95. https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000119
  • Üngör M. & Lachnit H. (2008). Dissociations among ABA ABC and AAB recovery effects. Learning and Motivation 39 181-195. http://doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2007.08.001
  • Vervliet B. Vansteenwegen D. Baeyens F. Hermans D. & Eelen P. (2005). Return of fear in a human differential conditioning paradigm caused by a stimulus change after extinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy 43 357-371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.005
  • Vila N. J. & Rosas J. M. (2001). Renewal and spontaneous recovery after extinction in a causal-learning task. Revista Mexicana De Análisis De La Conducta 27 79-96. Retrieved from http://rmac-mx.org/renewal-and-spontaneous-recovery-after-extinction-in-a-causal-learning-task/
  • Wagner A R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In N. E. Spear & R. R. Miller (Eds.) Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (pp. 5-47). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
  • Westbrook R F. Jones M. L. Bailey G. K. & Harris J. (2000). Contextual control over conditioned responding in a latent inhibition paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 26 157- 173. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.26.2.157