An integrative study of autobiographical memory for positive and negative experiences

  1. García Bajos, Elvira 1
  2. Migueles Seco, Malen 1
  1. 1 Universidad del País Vasco/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea
    info

    Universidad del País Vasco/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea

    Lejona, España

    ROR https://ror.org/000xsnr85

Revista:
The Spanish Journal of Psychology

ISSN: 1138-7416

Año de publicación: 2013

Número: 16

Páginas: 1-11

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.1017/SJP.2013.103 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: The Spanish Journal of Psychology

Resumen

In this study we examined autobiographical memory for emotional experiences from an integrative perspective, analyzing nature, distribution, content and phenomenological assessment of the experiences. Undergraduate students produced positive or negative life experiences, dated them and indicated their emotional level. Afterwards, they chose an experience to narrate, and rated their memory for the experience. The positive and negative experiences were grouped into 14 categories, although most of them fell into four areas: studies, family, friends and couple. The number of positive and negative experiences progressively increased from very few childhood memories to a larger percentage of more recent events. The distribution was equivalent, except during adolescence, in which a greater number of negative than positive experiences were recorded. The negative narratives included more emotional details, references to cognitive processes, mental rehearsal and justifications than the positive narratives. But the positive experiences obtained higher ratings in vividness and sensorial details than the negative experiences.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Berntsen D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered more frequently from shocking than for happy experiences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1010-1020. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194319.
  • Berntsen D., & Rubin D. C. (2002). Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories. Psychology and Aging, 17, 636-652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.636.
  • Berntsen D., Rubin D. C., & Siegler I. C. (2011). Two versions of life: Emotionally native and positive life events have different roles in the organization of life story and identity. Emotion, 11, 1190-1201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024940.
  • Boals A., Hathaway L. M., & Rubin D. C. (2011). The therapeutic effects of completing autobiographical memory questionnaires for positive and negative events: An experimental approach. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 544-549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9412-9.
  • Bluck S., & Habermas T. (2000). The life story schema. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 121-147.
  • Bohanek J. G., Fivush R., & Walker E. (2005). Memories of positive and negative emotional events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 51-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1064.
  • Butler L. D., & Wolfner A. L. (2000). Some characteristics of positive and negative ("most traumatic") event memories in a college sample. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 1, 45-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J229v01n01-04.
  • Byrne C. A., Hyman I. E., & Scott K. L. (2001). Comparisons of memories for traumatic events and other experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S119-S133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.837.
  • Christianson S. Å., & Loftus E. F. (1990). Some characteristics of people's traumatic memories. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 195-198.
  • Collins K. A., Pillemer D. B., Ivcevic Z., & Gooze R. A. (2007). Cultural scripts guide recall of intensely positive life events. Memory & Cognition, 35, 651-659. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193303.
  • Conway M. A. (1996). Autobiographical memory. In E. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp. 165-194). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Conway M. A., & Holmes A. (2004). Psychosocial stages and the accessibility of autobiographical memories across the life cycle. Journal of Personality, 72, 461-480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00269.x.
  • Conway M. A., Singer J. A., & Tagini A. (2004). The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 491-529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.5.491.50768.
  • Crovitz H. F., & Schiffman H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 517-518.
  • D'Argembeau A., Comblain C., & Van der Linden M. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for positive, negative, and neutral events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 281-294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.856.
  • D'Argembeau A., & Van der Linden M. (2004). Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844-858. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007.
  • Destun L. M., & Kuiper N. A. (1999). Phenomenal characteristics associated with real and imagined events: The effects of event valence and absorption. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 175-186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199904)13:2<175::AID-ACP552>3.3.CO;2-N.
  • Jansari A., & Parkin A. J. (1996). Things that go bump in your life: Explaining the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory. Psychology and Aging, 11, 85-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.11.1.85.
  • Leist A. K., Ferring D., & Filipp S.-H. (2010). Remembering positive and negative life events. Associations with future time perspective and functions of autobiographical memory. GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 137-147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000017.
  • Matlin M. W., & Stang D. J. (1978). The Pollyanna Principle. Selectivity in language, memory, and thought. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman.
  • Pillemer D. B., Rhinehart E. D., & White S. H. (1986). Memories of life transitions: The first year in college. Human Learning, 5, 109-123.
  • Porter S., & Birt A. R. (2001). Is traumatic memory special? A comparison of traumatic memory characteristics with memory for other emotional life experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S101-S117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.766.
  • Rasmussen A. S., & Berntsen D. (2009). Emotional valence and the functions of autobiographical memories: Positive and negative memories serve different functions. Memory & Cognition, 37, 477-492. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.4.477.
  • Rubin D. C. (1998). Autobiographical memory and aging: Distributions of memories across the life-span and their implications for survey research. In N. Schwarz, D. C. Park, B. Knäuper, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Cognition, aging and self-reports (pp. 163-183). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Rubin D. C., Rahhal T. A., & Poon L. W. (1998). Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best. Memory & Cognition, 26, 3-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03211366.
  • Rubin D. C., & Schulkind M. D. (1997). Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 524-535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.524.
  • Rubin D. C., Wetzler S. E., & Nebes R. D. (1986). Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 202-221). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Schacter D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54, 182-203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.54.3.182.
  • Schaefer A., & Philippot P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory, 13, 148-160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000648.
  • Schryer E., & Ross M. (2012). Evaluating the valence of remembered events: The importance of age and self-relevance. Psychology and Aging, 27, 237-242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023283.
  • Talarico J. M., Berntsen D., & Rubin D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 380-398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930801993999.
  • Tromp S., Koss M. P., Figueredo A. J., & Tharan M. (1995). Are rape memories different? A comparison of rape, other unpleasant, and peasant memories among employed women. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 607-627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02102891.
  • Walker W. R., Skowronski J. J., Gibbons J. A., Vogl R. J., & Thompson C. P. (2003). On the emotions that accompany autobiographical memories: Dysphoria disrupts the fading affect bias. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 703-723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930302287.
  • Walker W. R., Vogl R. J., & Thompson C. P. (1997). Autobiographical memory: Unpleasantness fades faster than pleasantness over time. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 399-413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199710)11:5<399::AID-ACP462>3.3.CO;2-5.
  • Walls R. T., Sperling R. A., & Weber K. D. (2001). Autobiographical memory of school. Journal of Educational Research, 95, 116-127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220670109596580.
  • Werner-Seidler A., & Moulds M. L. (2012). Mood repair and processing mode in depression. Emotion, 12, 470-478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025984.
  • Wessel I., & Merckelbach H. (1994). Characteristics of traumatic memories in normal subjects. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 315-324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465800013199.