Ethologically-determined responses of male mice in new dyadic encounters reflect their previous 'social status'

  1. Sánchez Martín, José Ramón
  2. Brain, Paul F.
  3. Fano Ardanaz, Eduardo
Revue:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915

Année de publication: 1997

Volumen: 9

Número: 2

Pages: 391-397

Type: Article

D'autres publications dans: Psicothema

Résumé

La conducta de ratones macho ante nuevas interacciones diádicas, evaluada etológicamente, refleja su ¿estatus social¿ previo. El presente estudio examina la importancia de la dominancia o sumisión en parejas de ratones, sobre su conducta en posteriores interacciones agonísticas. Los animales fueron alojados en parejas durante tres días y, en función de sus pautas de conducta (ataque, amenaza, sumisión, evitación) fueron clasificados como dominantes o subordinados. Posteriormente, se filmó la conducta de ratones dominantes enfrentados con dominantes, dominantes enfrentados con subordinados y subordinados enfrentados con subordinados, y tal conducta fue evaluada utilizando un sistema de análisis de tipo etológico. Tanto los animales sumisos como los dominantes mostraron características conductuales que reflejan su estatus social previo, y su conducta se vio menos influída por una evaluación inmediata del oponente. En el curso de este experimento, los animales dominantes mostraron relativamente pocas pautas de conducta defensiva o de evitación, independientemente del tipo de oponente. Por su parte, los animales sumisos, no mostraron conductas ofensivas cuando se enfrentaron con dominantes ni cuando lo hicieron con sumisos. Además, el estatus previo de los animales influyó sobre elementos conductuales no estrictamente agonísticos.

Références bibliographiques

  • Andrade, M.L., Kamal, B.H.K. and Brain, P.F. (1989). Effects of positive and negative fighting experience on behaviour . In Brain P.F., D. Mainardi and S. Parmigiani (eds.), House mouse aggression. A model for understanding the evolution of social behaviour (pp. 223- 232). Chur: Harwood Academic Publishers.
  • Benton, D. and Dalrymple-Alford, J. (1981). Social dominance in the mouse. In P.F. Brain P.F. and D. Benton (eds.), The biology of aggression (pp. 571-577). Alphen aan den Rijn: Sijthoff and Noordhoff.
  • Bernstein, I.S. (1980). Dominance: A theoretical perspective for ethologists. In D.R. Omark, F.F. Strayer and D.G. Freedman (eds.), Dominance relations (pp. 71-84). New York : Garland STPM Press.
  • Bernstein, I.S. (1981). Dominance: the baby and the bathwater. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 419-429.
  • Blanchard, R.J. and Blanchard, D.C. (1981). The organization and modelling of animal aggression. In P.F. Brain P.F. and D. Benton (eds.), The biology of aggression (pp. 529-561). Alphen aan den Rijn: Sijthoff and Noordhoff.
  • Blanchard, R.J. and Blanchard, D.C. (1986). Ethological models of fear and angry aggression. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology , 9, 383-385.
  • Brain, P.F. (1981). Differentiating types of attack and defense in rodents. In P.F. Brain and D. Benton (eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to aggression research (pp. 53-77). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland.
  • Brain, P.F. (1992). Understanding the behaviours of feral species may facilitate design of optimal living conditions for common laboratory rodents. Animal Technology, 43, 99-105.
  • Brain, P.F., McAllister, K.H. and Walmsley, S. (1989). Drug effects on social behavior. In A.A. Boulton, G.B. Baker and A.J. Greenshaw (eds.), Neuromethods, Vol. 13. Psychopharmacology (pp. 687-739). Clifton, New Jersey: The Humana Press Inc.
  • Brain, P.F. and Parmigiani, S. (1990). Variation in aggressiveness in house mouse populations. Biological Journal of Linnean Society, 41, 257-269
  • Brain, P.F. and Poole, A.E. (1974). Some studies on the use of ‘Standard opponents’ in intermale aggression testing in TT albino mice. Behaviour, 50, 100-110.
  • Ely, D.L. and Henry, J.P. (1978). Neuroendocrine response patterns in dominant and subordinate mice. Hormones and Behavior, 10, 156- 173.
  • Fernández-Espejo, E. and Mir, D. (1990). Ethological analysis of the male rat’s socioagonistic behavior in a resident-intruder paradigm. Aggressive Behavior, 16, 41-55.
  • Goldsmith, J.F., Brain, P.F. and Benton, D. (1976). Effects of age at differential housing and the durations of individual housing/grouping on intermale fighting behaviour and adrenocortical activity in ‘TO’ strain mice. Aggressive Behavior, 2, 307-323.
  • Kamal, K.B.H. (1986). Effects of experience and familiarity on social interactions of laboratory albino mice (Mus musculus L.). University of Wales: Ph. D. Dissertation.
  • Lagerspetz, K.M.J. (1964). Studies on the aggressive behaviour of mice. Annales Academiae Scientiarum Fennicae (B), 131, 1- 131.
  • Martínez, M., Castaño, D., Simón, V. and Brain, P.F. (1986). An ethopharmacological assessment of the influence of cyproterone acetate on social interactions in male mice. IRCS Medical Science, 14, 44-45.
  • Martínez, M., Salvador, A. and Simón, V. M. (1994). Behavioral changes over succesful agonistic encounters between male mice: Effects of type of “standard opponent”. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 441-451
  • Moyer, K.E. (1968). Kinds of aggression and their physiological basis. Communications in Behavioral Biology, 2, 65-87.
  • Parmigiani, S. and Brain, P.F. (1983). Effects of residence, aggressive experience and intruder familiarity on attack shown by male mice. Behavioural Processes, 8, 45-57.
  • Richards, S.M. (1974). The concept of dominance and methods of assessment. Animal Behaviour, 22, 914-930.
  • Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistic for the behavioral sciences. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill.
  • Syme , G.J. (1974). Competitive orders: a measure of social dominance. Animal Behaviour, 22, 931-940.
  • Van de Poll, N.E., de Jonge F., Van Oyen H.G. and Van Pelt J. (1982). Aggressive behaviour in rats; effects of wining and losing on subsequent aggressive interactions. Behavioral Processes, 7, 143-155.